TED演讲 Origins of pleasure by Paul Bloom 稿子

玛丽莲梦兔
842次浏览
2020年07月30日 23:46
最佳经验
本文由作者推荐

康复学-爱国卫生工作计划


The Origins of Pleasure
(by Paul Bloom)

I'm going to talk today about the pleasures of everyday life. But I want to begin
with a story of an unusual and terrible man. This is Hermann Goering. Goering was
Hitler's second in command in World War II, his designated successor. And like Hitler,
Goering fancied himself a collector of art. He went through Europe, through World
War II, stealing, extorting and occasionally buying various paintings for his collection.
And what he really wanted was something by Vermeer. Hitler had two of them, and he
didn't have any. So he finally found an art dealer, a Dutch art dealer named Han van
Meegeren, who sold him a wonderful Vermeer for the cost of what would now be 10
million dollars. And it was his favorite artwork ever.
World War II came to an end, and Goering was captured, tried at
Nuremberg and ultimately sentenced to death. Then the Allied forces went through
his collections and found the paintings and went after the people who sold it to him.
And at some point the Dutch police came into Amsterdam and arrested Van Meegeren.
Van Meegeren was charged with the crime of treason, which is itself punishable by
death. Six weeks into his prison sentence, van Meegeren confessed. But he didn't
confess to treason. He said,
myself; I'm a forger.
a canvas and some paint, and I will paint a Vermeer much better than I sold that
disgusting Nazi. I also need alcohol and morphine, because it's the only way I can
work.
the charges of treason were dropped. He had a lesser charge of forgery, got a year
sentence and died a hero to the Dutch people. There's a lot more to be said about van
Meegeren, but I want to turn now to Goering, who's pictured here being interrogated
at Nuremberg.
Now Goering was, by all accounts, a terrible man. Even for a Nazi, he was a
terrible man. His American interrogators described him as an amicable psychopath.
But you could feel sympathy for the reaction he had when he was told that his favorite
painting was actually a forgery. According to his biographer,
first time he had discovered there was evil in the world.
himself soon afterwards. He had discovered after all that the painting he thought was
this was actually that. It looked the same, but it had a different origin, it was a
different artwork.
It wasn't just him who was in for a shock. Once van Meegeren was on trial, he
couldn't stop talking. And he boasted about all the great masterpieces that he himself
had painted that were attributed to other artists. In particular, Supper at
Emmaus
would come [from] all over the world to see it -- was actually a forgery. It was not
that painting, but that painting. And when that was discovered, it lost all its value and
was taken away from the museum.

1


Why does this matter? I'm a psychologists -- why do origins matter so much?
Why do we respond so much to our knowledge of where something comes from?
Well there's an answer that many people would give. Many sociologists like Veblen
and Wolfe would argue that the reason why we take origins so seriously is because
we're snobs, because we're focused on status. Among other things, if you want to
show off how rich you are, how powerful you are, it's always better to own an original
than a forgery because there's always going to be fewer originals than forgeries. I
don't doubt that that plays some role, but what I want to convince you of today is that
there's something else going on. I want to convince you that humans are, to some
extent, natural born essentialists. What I mean by this is we don't just respond to
things as we see them, or feel them, or hear them. Rather, our response is conditioned
on our beliefs, about what they really are, what they came from, what they're made of,
what their hidden nature is. I want to suggest that this is true, not just for how we
think about things, but how we react to things.
So I want to suggest that pleasure is deep -- and that this isn't true just for higher
level pleasures like art, but even the most seemingly simple pleasures are affected by
our beliefs about hidden essences. So take food. Would you eat this? Well, a good
answer is,
Some of you would eat it if it's beef, but not pork. Few of you would eat it if it's a rat
or a human. Some of you would eat it only if it's a strangely colored piece of tofu.
That's not so surprising.
But what's more interesting is how it tastes to you will depend critically on what
you think you're eating. So one demonstration of this was done with young children.
How do you make children not just be more likely to eat carrots and drink milk, but to
get more pleasure from eating carrots and drinking milk -- to think they taste better?
It's simple, you tell them they're from McDonald's. They believe McDonald's food is
tastier, and it leads them to experience it as tastier.
How do you get adults to really enjoy wine? It's very simple: pour it from an
expensive bottle. There are now dozens, perhaps hundreds of studies showing that if
you believe you're drinking the expensive stuff, it tastes better to you. This was
recently done with a neuroscientific twist. They get people into a fMRI scanner, and
while they're lying there, through a tube, they get to sip wine. In front of them on a
screen is information about the wine. Everybody, of course, drinks exactly the same
wine. But if you believe you're drinking expensive stuff, parts of the brain associated
with pleasure and reward light up like a Christmas tree. It's not just that you say it's
more pleasurable, you say you like it more, you really experience it in a different way.
Or take sex. These are stimuli I've used in some of my studies. And if you
simply show people these pictures, they'll say these are fairly attractive people. But
how attractive you find them, how romantically moved you are by them, rests
critically on who you think you're looking at. You probably think the picture on the
left is male, the one on the right is female. If that belief turns out to be mistaken, it

2


will make a difference. (Laughter) It will make a difference if they turn out to be
much younger or much older than you think they are. It will make a difference if you
were to discover that the person you're looking at with lust is actually a disguised
version of your son or daughter, your mother or father. Knowing somebody's your kin
typically kills the interest. Maybe one of the most heartening findings from the
psychology of pleasure is there's more to looking good than your physical appearance.
If you like somebody, they look better to you. This is why spouses in happy marriages
tend to think that their husband or wife looks much better than anyone else thinks that
they do.
So Capgras syndrome is a disorder where you get a specific delusion. Sufferers
of Capgras syndrome believe that the people they love most in the world have been
replaced by perfect duplicates. Now often, a result of Capgras syndrome is tragic.
People have murdered those that they loved, believing that they were murdering an
imposter. But there's at least one case where Capgras syndrome had a happy ending.
This was recorded in 1931. described a woman with Capgras syndrome
who complained about her poorly endowed and sexually inadequate lover.
was before she got Capgras syndrome. After she got it,
she has discovered that he possessed a double who was rich, virile, handsome and
course, it was the same man, but she was seeing him in different
ways.
As a third example, consider consumer products. So one reason why you
might like something is its utility. You can put shoes on your feet; you can play golf
with golf clubs; and chewed up bubble gum doesn't do anything at all for you. But
each of these three objects has value above and beyond what it can do for you based
on its history. The golf clubs were owned by John F. Kennedy and sold for
three-quarters of a million dollars at auction. The bubble gum was chewed up by pop
star Britney Spears and sold for several hundreds of dollars. And in fact, there's a
thriving market in the partially eaten food of beloved people. (Laughter) The shoes
are perhaps the most valuable of all. According to an unconfirmed report, a Saudi
millionaire offered 10 million dollars for this pair of shoes. They were the ones
thrown at George Bush at an Iraqi press conference several years ago.
Now this attraction to objects doesn't just work for celebrity objects. Each one
of us, most people, have something in our life that's literally irreplaceable, in that it
has value because of its history -- maybe your wedding ring, maybe your child's baby
shoes -- so that if it was lost, you couldn't get it back. You could get something that
looked like it or felt like it, but you couldn't get the same object back. With my
colleagues George Newman and Gil Diesendruck, we've looked to see what sort of
factors, what sort of history, matters for the objects that people like. So in one of our
experiments, we asked people to name a famous person who they adored, a living
person they adored.

3


So one answer was George Clooney. Then we asked them,
you pay for George Clooney's sweater?
you would pay for a brand new sweater or a sweater owned by somebody who you
didn't adore. Then we asked other groups of subjects -- we gave them different
restrictions and different conditions. So for instance, we told some people,
can buy the sweater, but you can't tell anybody you own it, and you can't resell it.
That drops the value of it, suggesting that that's one reason why we like it. But what
really causes an effect is you tell people,
about it, but before it gets to you, it's thoroughly washed.
the value. As my wife put it,
So let's go back to art. I would love a Chagall. I love the work of Chagall. But I
don't want a duplicate, even if I can't tell the difference. That's not because, or it's not
simply because, I'm a snob and want to boast about having an original. Rather, it's
because I want something that has a specific history. In the case of artwork, the
history is special indeed. The philosopher Denis Dutton in his wonderful book
Art Instinct
about the human performance underlying its that could explain the
difference between an original and a forgery. They may look alike, but they have a
different history. The original is typically the product of a creative act, the forgery
isn't. I think this approach can explain differences in people's taste in art.
This is a work by Jackson Pollock. Who here likes the work of Jackson Pollock?
Okay. Who here, it does nothing for them? They just don't like it. I'm not going to
make a claim about who's right, but I will make an empirical claim about people's
intuitions, which is that, if you like the work of Jackson Pollock, you'll tend more so
than the people who don't like it to believe that these works are difficult to create, that
they require a lot of time and energy and creative energy. I use Jackson Pollock on
purpose as an example because there's a young American artist who paints very much
in the style of Jackson Pollock, and her work was worth many tens of thousands of
dollars -- in large part because she's a very young artist.
This is Marla Olmstead who did most of her work when she was three years
old. The interesting thing about Marla Olmstead is her family made the mistake of
inviting the television program 60 Minutes II into their house to film her painting.
And they then reported that her father was coaching her. When this came out on
television, the value of her art dropped to nothing. It was the same art, physically, but
the history had changed.
I've been focusing now on the visual arts, but I want to give two examples
from music. This is Joshua Bell, a very famous violinist. And the Washington Post
reporter Gene Weingarten decided to enlist him for an audacious experiment. The
question is: How much would people like Joshua Bell, the music of Joshua Bell, if
they didn't know they were listening to Joshua Bell? So he got Joshua Bell to take his
million dollar violin down to a Washington D.C. subway station and stand in the

4


corner and see how much money he would make. And here's a brief clip of this.
(Violin music) After being there for three-quarters of an hour, he made 32 dollars.
Not bad. It's also not good. Apparently to really enjoy the music of Joshua Bell, you
have to know you're listening to Joshua Bell. He actually made 20 dollars more than
that, but he didn't count it. Because this woman comes up -- you see at the end of the
video -- she comes up. She had heard him at the Library of Congress a few weeks
before at this extravagant black-tie affair. So she's stunned that he's standing in a
subway station. So she's struck with pity. She reaches into her purse and hands him a
20.
The second example from music is from John Cage's modernist composition,
As many of you know, this is the composition where the pianist sits at a
bench, opens up the piano and sits and does nothing for four minutes and 33 seconds
-- that period of silence. And people have different views on this. But what I want to
point out is you can buy this from iTunes. (Laughter) For a dollar 99, you can listen to
that silence, which is different than other forms of silence.
Now I've been talking so far about pleasure, but what I want to suggest is that
everything I've said applies as well to pain. And how you think about what you're
experiencing, your beliefs about the essence of it, affect how it hurts. One lovely
experiment was done by Kurt Gray and Dan Wegner. What they did was they hooked
up Harvard undergraduates to an electric shock machine. And they gave them a series
of painful electric shocks. So it was a series of five painful shocks. Half of them are
told that they're being given the shocks by somebody in another room, but the person
in the other room doesn't know they're giving them shocks. There's no malevolence,
they're just pressing a button. The first shock is recorded as very painful. The second
shock feels less painful, because you get a bit used to it. The third drops, the fourth,
the fifth. The pain gets less. In the other condition, they're told that the person in the
next room is shocking them on purpose -- knows they're shocking them. The first
shock hurts like hell. The second shock hurts just as much, and the third and the
fourth and the fifth. It hurts more if you believe somebody is doing it to you on
purpose.
The most extreme example of this is that in some cases, pain under the right
circumstances can transform into pleasure. Humans have this extraordinarily
interesting property that will often seek out low-level doses of pain in controlled
circumstances and take pleasure from it -- as in the eating of hot chili peppers and
roller coaster rides. The point was nicely summarized by the poet John Milton who
wrote,
heaven.
And I'll end with that. Thank you.(Applause)



5


The Origins of Pleasure
(by Paul Bloom)

I'm going to talk today about the pleasures of everyday life. But I want to begin
with a story of an unusual and terrible man. This is Hermann Goering. Goering was
Hitler's second in command in World War II, his designated successor. And like Hitler,
Goering fancied himself a collector of art. He went through Europe, through World
War II, stealing, extorting and occasionally buying various paintings for his collection.
And what he really wanted was something by Vermeer. Hitler had two of them, and he
didn't have any. So he finally found an art dealer, a Dutch art dealer named Han van
Meegeren, who sold him a wonderful Vermeer for the cost of what would now be 10
million dollars. And it was his favorite artwork ever.
World War II came to an end, and Goering was captured, tried at
Nuremberg and ultimately sentenced to death. Then the Allied forces went through
his collections and found the paintings and went after the people who sold it to him.
And at some point the Dutch police came into Amsterdam and arrested Van Meegeren.
Van Meegeren was charged with the crime of treason, which is itself punishable by
death. Six weeks into his prison sentence, van Meegeren confessed. But he didn't
confess to treason. He said,
myself; I'm a forger.
a canvas and some paint, and I will paint a Vermeer much better than I sold that
disgusting Nazi. I also need alcohol and morphine, because it's the only way I can
work.
the charges of treason were dropped. He had a lesser charge of forgery, got a year
sentence and died a hero to the Dutch people. There's a lot more to be said about van
Meegeren, but I want to turn now to Goering, who's pictured here being interrogated
at Nuremberg.
Now Goering was, by all accounts, a terrible man. Even for a Nazi, he was a
terrible man. His American interrogators described him as an amicable psychopath.
But you could feel sympathy for the reaction he had when he was told that his favorite
painting was actually a forgery. According to his biographer,
first time he had discovered there was evil in the world.
himself soon afterwards. He had discovered after all that the painting he thought was
this was actually that. It looked the same, but it had a different origin, it was a
different artwork.
It wasn't just him who was in for a shock. Once van Meegeren was on trial, he
couldn't stop talking. And he boasted about all the great masterpieces that he himself
had painted that were attributed to other artists. In particular, Supper at
Emmaus
would come [from] all over the world to see it -- was actually a forgery. It was not
that painting, but that painting. And when that was discovered, it lost all its value and
was taken away from the museum.

1


Why does this matter? I'm a psychologists -- why do origins matter so much?
Why do we respond so much to our knowledge of where something comes from?
Well there's an answer that many people would give. Many sociologists like Veblen
and Wolfe would argue that the reason why we take origins so seriously is because
we're snobs, because we're focused on status. Among other things, if you want to
show off how rich you are, how powerful you are, it's always better to own an original
than a forgery because there's always going to be fewer originals than forgeries. I
don't doubt that that plays some role, but what I want to convince you of today is that
there's something else going on. I want to convince you that humans are, to some
extent, natural born essentialists. What I mean by this is we don't just respond to
things as we see them, or feel them, or hear them. Rather, our response is conditioned
on our beliefs, about what they really are, what they came from, what they're made of,
what their hidden nature is. I want to suggest that this is true, not just for how we
think about things, but how we react to things.
So I want to suggest that pleasure is deep -- and that this isn't true just for higher
level pleasures like art, but even the most seemingly simple pleasures are affected by
our beliefs about hidden essences. So take food. Would you eat this? Well, a good
answer is,
Some of you would eat it if it's beef, but not pork. Few of you would eat it if it's a rat
or a human. Some of you would eat it only if it's a strangely colored piece of tofu.
That's not so surprising.
But what's more interesting is how it tastes to you will depend critically on what
you think you're eating. So one demonstration of this was done with young children.
How do you make children not just be more likely to eat carrots and drink milk, but to
get more pleasure from eating carrots and drinking milk -- to think they taste better?
It's simple, you tell them they're from McDonald's. They believe McDonald's food is
tastier, and it leads them to experience it as tastier.
How do you get adults to really enjoy wine? It's very simple: pour it from an
expensive bottle. There are now dozens, perhaps hundreds of studies showing that if
you believe you're drinking the expensive stuff, it tastes better to you. This was
recently done with a neuroscientific twist. They get people into a fMRI scanner, and
while they're lying there, through a tube, they get to sip wine. In front of them on a
screen is information about the wine. Everybody, of course, drinks exactly the same
wine. But if you believe you're drinking expensive stuff, parts of the brain associated
with pleasure and reward light up like a Christmas tree. It's not just that you say it's
more pleasurable, you say you like it more, you really experience it in a different way.
Or take sex. These are stimuli I've used in some of my studies. And if you
simply show people these pictures, they'll say these are fairly attractive people. But
how attractive you find them, how romantically moved you are by them, rests
critically on who you think you're looking at. You probably think the picture on the
left is male, the one on the right is female. If that belief turns out to be mistaken, it

2


will make a difference. (Laughter) It will make a difference if they turn out to be
much younger or much older than you think they are. It will make a difference if you
were to discover that the person you're looking at with lust is actually a disguised
version of your son or daughter, your mother or father. Knowing somebody's your kin
typically kills the interest. Maybe one of the most heartening findings from the
psychology of pleasure is there's more to looking good than your physical appearance.
If you like somebody, they look better to you. This is why spouses in happy marriages
tend to think that their husband or wife looks much better than anyone else thinks that
they do.
So Capgras syndrome is a disorder where you get a specific delusion. Sufferers
of Capgras syndrome believe that the people they love most in the world have been
replaced by perfect duplicates. Now often, a result of Capgras syndrome is tragic.
People have murdered those that they loved, believing that they were murdering an
imposter. But there's at least one case where Capgras syndrome had a happy ending.
This was recorded in 1931. described a woman with Capgras syndrome
who complained about her poorly endowed and sexually inadequate lover.
was before she got Capgras syndrome. After she got it,
she has discovered that he possessed a double who was rich, virile, handsome and
course, it was the same man, but she was seeing him in different
ways.
As a third example, consider consumer products. So one reason why you
might like something is its utility. You can put shoes on your feet; you can play golf
with golf clubs; and chewed up bubble gum doesn't do anything at all for you. But
each of these three objects has value above and beyond what it can do for you based
on its history. The golf clubs were owned by John F. Kennedy and sold for
three-quarters of a million dollars at auction. The bubble gum was chewed up by pop
star Britney Spears and sold for several hundreds of dollars. And in fact, there's a
thriving market in the partially eaten food of beloved people. (Laughter) The shoes
are perhaps the most valuable of all. According to an unconfirmed report, a Saudi
millionaire offered 10 million dollars for this pair of shoes. They were the ones
thrown at George Bush at an Iraqi press conference several years ago.
Now this attraction to objects doesn't just work for celebrity objects. Each one
of us, most people, have something in our life that's literally irreplaceable, in that it
has value because of its history -- maybe your wedding ring, maybe your child's baby
shoes -- so that if it was lost, you couldn't get it back. You could get something that
looked like it or felt like it, but you couldn't get the same object back. With my
colleagues George Newman and Gil Diesendruck, we've looked to see what sort of
factors, what sort of history, matters for the objects that people like. So in one of our
experiments, we asked people to name a famous person who they adored, a living
person they adored.

3


So one answer was George Clooney. Then we asked them,
you pay for George Clooney's sweater?
you would pay for a brand new sweater or a sweater owned by somebody who you
didn't adore. Then we asked other groups of subjects -- we gave them different
restrictions and different conditions. So for instance, we told some people,
can buy the sweater, but you can't tell anybody you own it, and you can't resell it.
That drops the value of it, suggesting that that's one reason why we like it. But what
really causes an effect is you tell people,
about it, but before it gets to you, it's thoroughly washed.
the value. As my wife put it,
So let's go back to art. I would love a Chagall. I love the work of Chagall. But I
don't want a duplicate, even if I can't tell the difference. That's not because, or it's not
simply because, I'm a snob and want to boast about having an original. Rather, it's
because I want something that has a specific history. In the case of artwork, the
history is special indeed. The philosopher Denis Dutton in his wonderful book
Art Instinct
about the human performance underlying its that could explain the
difference between an original and a forgery. They may look alike, but they have a
different history. The original is typically the product of a creative act, the forgery
isn't. I think this approach can explain differences in people's taste in art.
This is a work by Jackson Pollock. Who here likes the work of Jackson Pollock?
Okay. Who here, it does nothing for them? They just don't like it. I'm not going to
make a claim about who's right, but I will make an empirical claim about people's
intuitions, which is that, if you like the work of Jackson Pollock, you'll tend more so
than the people who don't like it to believe that these works are difficult to create, that
they require a lot of time and energy and creative energy. I use Jackson Pollock on
purpose as an example because there's a young American artist who paints very much
in the style of Jackson Pollock, and her work was worth many tens of thousands of
dollars -- in large part because she's a very young artist.
This is Marla Olmstead who did most of her work when she was three years
old. The interesting thing about Marla Olmstead is her family made the mistake of
inviting the television program 60 Minutes II into their house to film her painting.
And they then reported that her father was coaching her. When this came out on
television, the value of her art dropped to nothing. It was the same art, physically, but
the history had changed.
I've been focusing now on the visual arts, but I want to give two examples
from music. This is Joshua Bell, a very famous violinist. And the Washington Post
reporter Gene Weingarten decided to enlist him for an audacious experiment. The
question is: How much would people like Joshua Bell, the music of Joshua Bell, if
they didn't know they were listening to Joshua Bell? So he got Joshua Bell to take his
million dollar violin down to a Washington D.C. subway station and stand in the

4


corner and see how much money he would make. And here's a brief clip of this.
(Violin music) After being there for three-quarters of an hour, he made 32 dollars.
Not bad. It's also not good. Apparently to really enjoy the music of Joshua Bell, you
have to know you're listening to Joshua Bell. He actually made 20 dollars more than
that, but he didn't count it. Because this woman comes up -- you see at the end of the
video -- she comes up. She had heard him at the Library of Congress a few weeks
before at this extravagant black-tie affair. So she's stunned that he's standing in a
subway station. So she's struck with pity. She reaches into her purse and hands him a
20.
The second example from music is from John Cage's modernist composition,
As many of you know, this is the composition where the pianist sits at a
bench, opens up the piano and sits and does nothing for four minutes and 33 seconds
-- that period of silence. And people have different views on this. But what I want to
point out is you can buy this from iTunes. (Laughter) For a dollar 99, you can listen to
that silence, which is different than other forms of silence.
Now I've been talking so far about pleasure, but what I want to suggest is that
everything I've said applies as well to pain. And how you think about what you're
experiencing, your beliefs about the essence of it, affect how it hurts. One lovely
experiment was done by Kurt Gray and Dan Wegner. What they did was they hooked
up Harvard undergraduates to an electric shock machine. And they gave them a series
of painful electric shocks. So it was a series of five painful shocks. Half of them are
told that they're being given the shocks by somebody in another room, but the person
in the other room doesn't know they're giving them shocks. There's no malevolence,
they're just pressing a button. The first shock is recorded as very painful. The second
shock feels less painful, because you get a bit used to it. The third drops, the fourth,
the fifth. The pain gets less. In the other condition, they're told that the person in the
next room is shocking them on purpose -- knows they're shocking them. The first
shock hurts like hell. The second shock hurts just as much, and the third and the
fourth and the fifth. It hurts more if you believe somebody is doing it to you on
purpose.
The most extreme example of this is that in some cases, pain under the right
circumstances can transform into pleasure. Humans have this extraordinarily
interesting property that will often seek out low-level doses of pain in controlled
circumstances and take pleasure from it -- as in the eating of hot chili peppers and
roller coaster rides. The point was nicely summarized by the poet John Milton who
wrote,
heaven.
And I'll end with that. Thank you.(Applause)



5

乐山技术学院-加工合同范本


阜阳市职业技术学院-学前班班主任工作总结


湖南女子学院官网-月工作总结ppt


生意经-外国人眼中的中国


本科院校排名-云南人事考试


曼德拉语录-财务述职报告


辽宁广告职业学院-东北师大录取分数线


关于这就是我的作文-四年级上册英语教案